Schmitt - Lexical chunks.pdf

(112 KB) Pobierz
<!DOCTYPE html PUBLIC "-//W3C//DTD HTML 4.01//EN" "http://www.w3.org/TR/html4/strict.dtd">
chunks are 'ready to go', and require little or no
additional processing.
Some lexical chunks have 'slots' which can take
different words according to the situation, provid-
ing a scaffold for quick, but flexible, language use.
For example, ' (person) thinks nothing of
ing (verb)' can provide the preformulated
platform for many different realizations, such as
Diane thinks nothing of hiking 20 miles, or He
thinks nothing of teaching six classes a day. The
ability to use preformed lexical chunks allows
greater fluency in speech production. The use of
lexical chunks can aid the listener as well. Because
lexical chunks can be recognized as individual
wholes, this spares the listener some of the
processing effort required to interpret an utter-
ance word-by-word.
It has been argued that lexical chunks also play a
part in vocabulary and grammar acquisition. Once
a chunk is known, it can be analysed and
segmented into its constituent words. This can
occur when some variability is noticed in a lexical
chunk. For example, after having heard the phrase
How are you today? several times, it may be
acquired as a chunk with the meaning of 'a
greeting'. However, the learner may later notice
the phrases How are you this evening? or How are
you this fine morning?. At that point, the learner
may realize that the underlying structure is
actually How are you ?, where the slot
can be filled with a time reference. The learner is
then aware that what fits in the slot is a separate
unit from the rest of the phrase, which opens the
door to learning that lexical unit. Eventually, the
entire lexical chunk may be analysed into separate
words, although it may continue to be stored as a
whole because of its utility. Because this segmen-
tation also involves syntax, it has been suggested
that it can also lead to grammatical acquisition.
Norbert Schmitt, University of Nottingham
Lexical chunks
Although vocabulary has conventionally been
conceptualized as individual words, it has now
become clear that much of lexis consists of
sequences of words which operate as single
units. Traditional approaches have long dealt
with multi-word units (MWU), wherein a single
meaning is attached to more than one word, for
example, phrasal verbs ('give up'), compounds
('freeze-dry'), and idioms ('burn the midnight
oil'). But corpus-based research has shown that
collocation (the tendency for words to occur
together in discourse) extends far beyond the
level of such MWUs. In fact, it appears quite
common for longer sequences of words to pattern
together. Some of these recur frequently enough
to be treated as units in their own right, e.g. to
make a long story short. Numerous terms have
been coined to refer to this type of sequence, but
the most commonly used are lexical chunks and
lexical phrases.
One reason these lexical chunks are so common is
that they are typically related to functional
language use. For example, the above lexical
chunk is often used in summarizing, while Have
you heard the one about ? is reserved for
beginning a joke or humorous story. Lexical
chunks like these are institutionalized as the
most efficient and most familiar linguistic means
to carry out language functions. As such, they
facilitate clear, relevant, and concise language use.
Because of their functional usage, knowledge of
lexical chunks is essential for pragmatic compe-
tence.
There is a good psycholinguistic basis for believing
that the mind stores and processes these chunks as
individual wholes. The main reason stems from
the structure of the mind itself. It can store vast
amounts of knowledge in long-term memory, but
is only able to process small amounts of it in real-
time, such as when one is speaking. In effect, the
mind makes use of a relatively abundant resource
(long-term memory) to compensate for a relative
lack in another (processing capacity) by storing a
number of frequently-needed lexical chunks as
individual whole units. These can be easily
retrieved and used without the need to compose
them on-line through word selection and gram-
matical sequencing. This means there is less
demand on cognitive capacity, because the lexical
References
Bolinger, D. 1976. 'Meaning and memory'. Forum
Linguisticum 1: 1-14.
Moon, R. 1997. 'Vocabulary connections: Multi-
word items in English' in N. Schmitt and M.
McCarthy (eds.). Vocabulary: Description,
Acquisition, and Pedagogy. Cambridge: Cam-
bridge University Press.
ELT Journal Volume 54/4 October 2000 © Oxford University Press 2000
400
838956814.001.png
Nattinger, J. R. and J. S. DeCarrico. (1992). Sinclair, J. (1991). Corpus, Concordance, Colloca-
Lexical Phrases and Language Teaching.
tion. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Pawley, A. and F. H. Syder. (1983). 'Two puzzles
for linguistic theory: Native-like selection and
aut hor
'" e
Norbe rt Schmitt lectures at the University of
native-like fluency' in J Richards and R.
Schmidt (eds.). Language and Communication.
Nottingham, and is interested in all aspects of L2
London: Longman.
vocabulary studies. He recently published Voca-
Peters, A. (1983). The Units of Language
bulary in Language Teaching with Cambridge
Acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University
University Press. He also hopes to find time to
Press.
start playing his bagpipes again.
Key concepts in ELT
401
838956814.002.png
Zgłoś jeśli naruszono regulamin